Research Article

Multiscale representation of very large environments in the hippocampus of flying bats

See allHide authors and affiliations

Science  28 May 2021:
Vol. 372, Issue 6545, eabg4020
DOI: 10.1126/science.abg4020

You are currently viewing the abstract.

View Full Text

Log in to view the full text

Log in through your institution

Log in through your institution

Coding for space in the mammalian brain

Nearly all mammals navigate over large spatial scales in environments that span hundreds of meters to many kilometers. However, very little is known about the neural representations that underlie the coding of such large spaces. Eliav et al. recorded from place cells in the hippocampus of bats as they flew back and forth on an extremely long track (see the Perspective by Wood and Dudchenko). Many place cells had multiple place fields within this large environment. The place field sizes ranged from less than 1 meter up to 32 meters, and the sizes of the different place fields of an individual cell varied as much as 20-fold. Studying animals under naturalistic conditions can reveal new coding principles for the representation of their environment in the brain.

Science, abg4020, this issue p. eabg4020; see also abi9663, p. 913

Structured Abstract

INTRODUCTION

Place cells are neurons in the hippocampus that represent the animal’s position in space and are important for supporting navigation behaviors. These cells increase their spiking activity when the animal passes through a specific region of space, called the neuron’s “place field.” Since the discovery of place cells half a century ago, nearly all the research on spatial representations in the mammalian brain has focused on rats and mice as animal models and used small laboratory environments as experimental setups—usually small boxes or short linear tracks ~1 to 2 m in size. In such small environments, individual place cells typically have one place field, with a small field size. However, outdoor navigation of all mammals occurs in natural environments that span much larger spatial scales, of hundreds of meters or kilometers, and nothing is known about the neural codes for such large spatial scales.

RATIONALE

We reasoned that in very large environments, the hippocampus must exhibit a different coding scheme than seen in small environments because large environments cannot be tiled fully by the limited number of hippocampal neurons. We set out to discover this alternative coding scheme and thus to close the longstanding gap between the neurobiology of navigation as studied in the laboratory and natural large-scale navigation. To this end, we studied bats flying in a 200-m-long tunnel while we recorded the activity of hippocampal dorsal CA1 neurons using a custom wireless-electrophysiology system.

RESULTS

We found that place cells recorded in the large environment exhibited a multifield, multiscale representation of space: Individual neurons exhibited multiple place fields of diverse sizes, ranging from <1 m to more than 30 m, and the fields of the same neuron could differ up to 20-fold in size. This multifield, multiscale code was observed already from the first day in the environment and was similar between wild-born and laboratory-born bats that were never exposed to large environments. By contrast, recordings from a small-scale 6-m environment did not reveal such a multiscale code but rather classical single fields. Theoretical decoding analysis showed major advantages of the multiscale code over classical single-field codes, both in the number of required neurons and in the decoding errors. Thus, the multiscale code provides an efficient population code with a high capacity for representing very large environments. We conducted neural-network modeling, which suggested that the multiscale code may arise from interacting attractor networks with multiple scales or from feedforward networks, which yielded experimentally testable predictions for the inputs into CA1.

CONCLUSION

Using this experimental setup, our study uncovered a new coding scheme for large spaces, which was never observed before in small spaces: a multiscale code for space. This coding scheme existed from day 1 in the environment and was observed in both wild-born and laboratory-born bats, suggesting that it does not require previous experience. These findings provide a new notion for how the hippocampus represents space. The large naturalistic scale of our experimental environment was crucial for revealing this type of code. More generally, this study demonstrates the power of studying brain circuits under naturalistic conditions.

Multiscale hippocampal spatial code for very large environments.

(Methods) We wirelessly recorded neural activity from hippocampal neurons of bats flying in a 200-m tunnel. (Findings) Single neurons exhibited multiple place fields with highly heterogeneous field sizes for the same neuron. (Function) This multiscale neural code for space strongly outperforms classical single-field place codes. (Modeling) Modeling by using interacting attractor networks and feedforward models recapitulated the multiscale coding.

Abstract

Hippocampal place cells encode the animal’s location. Place cells were traditionally studied in small environments, and nothing is known about large ethologically relevant spatial scales. We wirelessly recorded from hippocampal dorsal CA1 neurons of wild-born bats flying in a long tunnel (200 meters). The size of place fields ranged from 0.6 to 32 meters. Individual place cells exhibited multiple fields and a multiscale representation: Place fields of the same neuron differed up to 20-fold in size. This multiscale coding was observed from the first day of exposure to the environment, and also in laboratory-born bats that never experienced large environments. Theoretical decoding analysis showed that the multiscale code allows representation of very large environments with much higher precision than that of other codes. Together, by increasing the spatial scale, we discovered a neural code that is radically different from classical place codes.

View Full Text

Stay Connected to Science